Baayen, H., Davidson, D., Bates, D. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390-412. DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005 Baguley, T. (2012). Calculating and graphing within-subject confidence intervals for ANOVA. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 158-175. DOI: 10.3758/s13428-011-0123-7 Barr, D., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., Tily, H. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255-278. DOI: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 Belia, S, Fidler, F., Williams, J., Cumming, G. (2005). Researchers misunderstand confidence intervals and standard error bars. Psychological Methocs, 10, 389-396. DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.389. Blouin, D., Rioppele, A. (2005). On confidence intervals for within-subjects designs. Psychological Methods, 10, 397-412. DOI: 10.1037/1082-989X.10.4.397 Chang, Y., Lane, D. (2016). Generalizing across stimuli as well as subjects: a non-mathematical tutorial on mixed-effects models. The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 12, 201-219. DOI: 10.20982/tqmp.12.3.p201 Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: a simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 1, 42-45. DOI: 10.20982/tqmp.01.1.p042 Cumming, G., (2014). The new statistics: why and how. Psychological Science, 25, 7-29. DOI: 10.1177/0956797613504966 Cumming, G., Finch, S. (2005). Inference by eye: confidence intervals and how to read pictures of data. American Psychologist, 60, 170-180. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.60.2.170 Davison, A., Hinkley, D. (1997). Bootstrap Methods and Their Application. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Franz, V., Loftus, G. (2012). Standard errors and confidence intervals in within-subjects designs: generalizing Loftus and Masson (1994) and avoiding the biases of alternative accounts. Psychonomic Bulletin \& Review, 19, 395-404. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-012-0230-1 Gould, L., McKibben, T., Ekstrand, C., Lorentz, E., Borowsky, R. (2016). The beat goes on: the effect of rhythm on reading aloud. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31, 236-250. Judd, C., Westfall, J., Kenny, D. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: a new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 54-69. DOI: 10.1037/a0028347 Loftus, G., Masson, M. (1994). Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 1, 476-490. DOI: 10.3758/BF03210951 Morey, R. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: a correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4, 61-64. DOI: 10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061 Morey, R., Hoekstra, R., Rouder, M., Wagenmakers, E. (2016). The fallacy of placing confidence in confidence intervals. Psychonomic Bulletin \& Review, 23, 103-123. DOI: 10.3758/s13423-015-0947-8 Pfister, R., Jancyzk, M. (2013). Confidence intervals for two sample means: calculation, interpretation, and a few simple rules. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 9, 74-80. DOI: 10.5709/acp-0133-x Rouder, J., Morey, R. (2005). Relational and arelational confidence intervals: a comment on Fidler, Thomason, Cumming, Finch, and Leeman (2004). Psychological Science, 16, 77-79). DOI: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00783.x Schaalje, G., McBride, J., Fellingham, G. (2002). Adequacy of approximations to distributions of test statistics in complex mixed linear models. Journal of Agricultural, Biological, and Environmental Statistics, 7, 512-524. DOI: 10.1198/108571102726